The Right Side of Wrong: Karen Read Trials

Bayesian Evidence Analyzer

Verdict (June 18, 2025): Not Guilty of Murder, Convicted of OUI

Imagine you're a detective trying to solve a mystery. You start with some initial ideas, but as you find clues, your ideas change? Bayesian analysis is a math tool that helps us do exactly that – update our beliefs as we get new information (or 'evidence').

It's named after Thomas Bayes, who came up with the main idea in the 18th Century. We use it in science, medicine, and even in everyday thinking without realizing it!

Our Big Questions (Hypotheses)

In the Karen Read trial, we're looking at two main possibilities (these are our 'hypotheses'):

Your Starting Belief (Prior Probability)

Before we look at any specific evidence from the trial, what's your gut feeling? This starting belief is called your 'prior probability.' For example, you might think it's 50/50, or you might lean one way or the other. This tool will let you set your starting point.

How Strong is Each Clue? (Likelihoods)

For every piece of evidence (each 'Fact' from the trial), we ask two questions:

These are called 'likelihoods.' Some clues might strongly point to H1, others to H2, and some might not be very strong either way. You'll get to estimate these for each fact.

Updating Your Beliefs

Once you set your starting belief and estimate how strong each clue is, the Bayesian Analyzer will use math (Bayes' Theorem) to calculate a new 'updated belief' (called a 'posterior probability'). This shows how your belief might change after considering all the evidence you've rated.

The Logic & The Math (An In-Depth Look)

Thomas Bayes (c. 1701 – 17 April 1761) was an English statistician, philosopher, and Presbyterian minister who first formulated the theorem. You can learn more about him on his Wikipedia page.

The Theorem Mathematically:

P(H|E) =
P(E|H) × P(H)
P(E)

Let's break down what these terms mean in the context of our analyzer:

  • P(H|E): This is the Posterior Probability. It's what we want to find – the probability of our hypothesis (H) being true after considering the new evidence (E).
  • P(E|H): This is the Likelihood. It's the probability of observing the evidence (E) if our hypothesis (H) were true. You set this with the sliders for each fact.
  • P(H): This is the Prior Probability. It's our initial belief in the hypothesis (H) being true before considering the new evidence (E). You set this with the 'Your Starting Belief' slider.
  • P(E): This is the Probability of the Evidence. It's the overall probability of observing the evidence (E) under all possible hypotheses. It's calculated in the background to ensure the final probability is correct.

The Logic: Why Does It Work?

Bayes' Theorem provides a rational way to update beliefs. If evidence is more likely given your hypothesis is true, your belief in the hypothesis gets stronger. If the evidence is less likely, your belief gets weaker.

Everyday Applications of Bayesian Reasoning:

  • Medical Diagnosis: Doctors update the probability of a patient having a disease based on symptoms and test results.
  • Spam Filters: Email services use Bayesian filtering to determine if an email is spam based on words and other features.
  • Legal Reasoning (informally): Jurors and investigators constantly update their beliefs about guilt or innocence as new pieces of evidence are presented. This tool makes that process explicit.

Key Considerations, Assumptions, and Educational Purpose:

  • Educational Focus: This site is intended to help us explore how reasonable people, starting from different assumptions or weighing evidence differently, can reach very different conclusions about the same case. By making our reasoning explicit, we can better understand how and why viewpoints diverge—even when everyone is acting in good faith. This is an educational exploration of Bayesian reasoning, not a verdict predictor.
  • Subjectivity is Inherent: Your 'Prior' and 'Likelihood' estimates are personal judgments. The output directly reflects your inputs.
  • Model Simplifications: The model assumes each piece of evidence is independent for simplicity. In reality, facts can be interconnected.

Step 1. Your Starting Belief (Prior Probability)

Before looking at the specific facts, decide how much you think Hypothesis 1 is correct. H1 is the claim that Karen Read caused John O’Keefe’s death. A setting of 0% means you think that claim is impossible, 50% means you view H1 and H2 as equally plausible, and 100% means you are certain H1 is true.

You might base this on your initial reaction when first hearing about the case, your impression after following media coverage, or you can start from a neutral 50/50 stance to see how the evidence shifts your belief. Whatever value you choose for H1, your belief in H2 is simply 100% minus that amount.

50%

This means your starting belief in H2 (Karen Read is NOT responsible) is: 50%

Step 2. Rate the Strength of Each Piece of Evidence (Likelihoods)

For each fact below, we're evaluating how strongly it supports either hypothesis. For each fact, consider:

  1. If Karen Read is responsible (H1): How likely would we expect to see this specific fact? (0% = would never happen if she's guilty, 100% = would definitely happen if she's guilty)
  2. If Karen Read is NOT responsible (H2): How likely would we expect to see this specific fact? (0% = would never happen if she's innocent, 100% = would definitely happen if she's innocent)

How to interpret the likelihood sliders:

  • 0–1×: Weak evidence
  • 5–7×: Strong evidence
  • 10×: Very strong/conclusive
  • Values in between represent varying degrees of support
💡 Tip: Play with the sliders and priors to see how your beliefs change! Bayesian reasoning is about making your assumptions explicit and seeing how conclusions shift.
How Bayesian Updating Works:
Prior Evidence Posterior
Start with a prior belief → update with evidence → get a new belief (posterior)
Show an Example: Numeric Walkthrough
Example: Suppose your prior for H1 is 20%, and you rate a piece of evidence as 80% likely if H1 is true, and 40% likely if H2 is true.
  • Prior odds for H1:H2 = 20:80 = 0.25
  • Likelihood ratio = 80/40 = 2
  • Posterior odds = 0.25 × 2 = 0.5
  • Posterior probability for H1 = 0.5 / (0.5 + 1) = 33%
This is how a single piece of evidence updates your belief!

Fact 1: John O'Keefe found dead outside 34 Fairview Rd, ~6 a.m., Jan 29, 2022, during snowstorm

Analysis: This fact is not in dispute, but we need to consider how likely it is under each hypothesis.

  • If Karen is responsible (H1), we'd expect to see this fact with high probability (e.g., 95%)
  • If Karen is not responsible (H2), we'd also expect to see this fact with high probability (e.g., 95%)
  • Since the ratio is 1:1, this fact doesn't change our belief in either direction
95%
95%

Fact 2: Blunt-force head injuries, hypothermia, abrasions on right arm

Analysis: This pattern of injuries needs to be evaluated under each hypothesis.

  • If Karen is responsible (H1), these injuries could be from a vehicle impact (e.g., 70% likely)
  • If Karen is not responsible (H2), these could be from a fight or fall (e.g., 40% likely)
  • This makes the injuries about 1.75x more likely under H1 than H2
70%
40%

Fact 3: Broken Lexus taillight; matching plastic on lawn & clothes

Analysis: This is a key piece of physical evidence that needs careful consideration.

  • If Karen is responsible (H1), this evidence is highly consistent with a vehicle impact (e.g., 85% likely)
  • If Karen is not responsible (H2), this would be more coincidental (e.g., 20% likely)
  • This makes the evidence about 4.25x more likely under H1 than H2
85%
20%

Fact 4: Vehicle data: reverse shift & 24 mph spike at 12:45 a.m.

75%
25%

Analysis: The vehicle data shows a reverse shift and speed spike that could indicate a collision. If Karen was responsible, this might represent her backing into the victim (high probability, ~75%). If she wasn't responsible, this could be explained by normal driving patterns or other activities (lower probability, ~25%).

Fact 5: Witnesses (EMT, Jen McCabe) heard Read say "I hit him"

85%
30%

Analysis: If Karen was responsible, she might admit it in a moment of distress (high probability, ~85%). If she wasn't responsible, she might still say this if confused, coerced, or referring to something else (moderate probability, ~30%).

Fact 6: Google search "how long to die in cold"

70%
15%

Analysis: This search would be more likely if Karen was responsible and trying to understand potential outcomes (high probability, ~70%). If she wasn't responsible, this search would be unusual unless there was another explanation (low probability, ~15%).


Fact 7: No blood on car bumper, despite hair/DNA transfer

30%
70%

Analysis: The absence of blood on the bumper is unexpected if Karen hit the victim with enough force to cause fatal injuries (low probability under H1, ~30%). If she didn't hit the victim, the lack of blood is more consistent (higher probability under H2, ~70%). The hair/DNA transfer could have occurred without a forceful impact.


Fact 8: Defense claims: No blood on taillight pieces

25%
75%

Analysis: If the taillight broke during a fatal collision, blood evidence would be expected (low probability under H1, ~25%). The absence of blood suggests the taillight may have broken under different circumstances (higher probability under H2, ~75%). This supports the defense's argument that the damage wasn't from hitting the victim.


Fact 9: Defense claims: No broken glass on victim's clothing

20%
80%

Analysis: If the victim was hit with enough force to shatter the taillight, glass fragments would likely be found on their clothing (very low probability under H1, ~20%). The absence of glass strongly suggests the taillight wasn't broken during a collision with the victim (high probability under H2, ~80%).


Fact 10: Defense claims: No injuries consistent with car impact

15%
85%

Analysis: A vehicle impact severe enough to be fatal would typically leave clear impact injuries (very low probability under H1, ~15%). The absence of such injuries strongly contradicts the prosecution's theory (very high probability under H2, ~85%). This is a key point for the defense.


Fact 11: Defense claims: No clothing fibers on car

25%
75%

Analysis: A forceful impact would likely leave clothing fibers on the vehicle (low probability under H1, ~25%). The absence of fibers suggests no direct contact with clothing (high probability under H2, ~75%). This absence weakens the prosecution's physical evidence case.


Fact 12: Defense claims: No DNA on car exterior

20%
80%

Analysis: A fatal collision would likely transfer DNA to the vehicle (low probability under H1, ~20%). The absence of DNA is more consistent with no direct contact (high probability under H2, ~80%). This is another significant gap in the physical evidence against Karen Read.


Fact 13: Defense claims: No blood on car exterior

20%
80%

Analysis: The complete absence of blood on the car's exterior is highly inconsistent with a fatal collision (very low probability under H1, ~20%). This strongly supports the defense's position that no impact occurred (high probability under H2, ~80%). The prosecution would need to explain how a fatal impact left no blood evidence.


Fact 14: Defense claims: No damage to car's undercarriage

25%
75%

Analysis: If the victim was struck with enough force to be fatal, significant undercarriage damage would be expected (low probability under H1, ~25%). The absence of such damage supports the defense's argument that no collision occurred (high probability under H2, ~75%). This is particularly significant given the alleged force needed to cause the victim's injuries.


Fact 15: Defense claims: No blood in wheel wells or undercarriage

15%
85%

Analysis: The complete absence of blood in the wheel wells or undercarriage is extremely unlikely if the victim was run over (very low probability under H1, ~15%). This is a critical piece of evidence supporting the defense's case (very high probability under H2, ~85%). Blood would be expected in these areas if the car had driven over the victim with enough force to cause fatal injuries.


Fact 17: Taillight evidence re-examination

40%
60%

Analysis: The re-examination of taillight evidence suggests the damage pattern is not fully consistent with a pedestrian impact (moderate probability under H1, ~40%). The findings are more consistent with alternative explanations like a pre-existing condition or unrelated damage (slightly higher probability under H2, ~60%). This is a relatively minor point compared to other physical evidence.


Fact 18: Colin Albert's testimony and text messages

30%
70%

Analysis: The content and timing of Colin Albert's testimony and text messages raise questions about potential witness coordination (low probability under H1, ~30%). The inconsistencies and timing of these communications are more consistent with a narrative being shaped after the fact (higher probability under H2, ~70%). This is particularly significant given Albert's relationship to law enforcement and potential biases.

Updated Belief in H1 (Read is responsible): --%

Updated Belief in H2 (Read is NOT responsible): --%

Part 3: Discussion

The Logic: Why Does It Work?

Bayes' Theorem provides a rational way to update beliefs. Here's the core idea:

  • Start with a belief: You have an initial idea about how likely something is (your prior probability, P(H)).
  • Get new evidence: You encounter a new piece of information (E).
  • Assess the evidence: You consider how likely this evidence would be if your initial idea was true (the likelihood, P(E|H)), and also how likely it would be if your initial idea was false (e.g., P(E|not H)).
  • Update your belief: The theorem combines your prior belief with the strength of the new evidence to give you a revised, more informed belief (the posterior probability, P(H|E)).

Essentially, if the evidence is more likely under your hypothesis than under alternative hypotheses, your belief in your hypothesis increases. If it's less likely, your belief decreases. The P(E) term acts as a normalizing factor, ensuring the resulting probability is valid (between 0 and 1).

Bayes' Theorem is a fundamental concept in probability theory that describes how to update the probability of a hypothesis based on new evidence. It's a powerful tool for reasoning under uncertainty.

The Theorem Mathematically:

P(H|E) = P(E|H) × P(H)P(E)

Let's break down what these terms mean in the context of our analyzer:

  • P(H|E): This is the Posterior Probability. It's what we want to find – the probability of our hypothesis (H) being true after considering the new evidence (E).
    In our case: P(H1|Fact) – The probability that Hypothesis 1 (Karen Read is responsible) is true, given a specific Fact from the trial.
  • P(E|H): This is the Likelihood. It's the probability of observing the evidence (E) if our hypothesis (H) were true. You set this with the sliders for each fact (e.g., "P(Fact | H1)").
    In our case: P(Fact|H1) – If Karen Read *is* responsible, how likely is this specific Fact?
  • P(H): This is the Prior Probability. It's our initial belief in the hypothesis (H) being true before considering the new evidence (E). You set this with the "Your Starting Belief" slider.
    In our case: P(H1) – Your initial belief that Karen Read is responsible, before looking at any specific trial evidence.
  • P(E): This is the Probability of the Evidence (also sometimes called the marginal likelihood). It's the overall probability of observing the evidence (E) under all possible hypotheses. It's calculated as:
    P(E) = P(E|H1) * P(H1) + P(E|H2) * P(H2)
    In our case: This is the probability of observing a specific Fact, considering both the scenario where H1 is true and the scenario where H2 (Karen Read is NOT responsible) is true. The analyzer calculates this in the background.

Important: This is an educational tool to help understand how evidence can shift perspectives. It's NOT predicting the actual jury outcome or saying what's true. The numbers you put in are your own estimates!

The main takeaway should be an understanding of how beliefs can be systematically adjusted in light of new information.

Everyday Applications of Bayesian Reasoning:

While the math might seem complex, Bayesian principles are used (often intuitively) in many real-world situations:

  • Medical Diagnosis: Doctors update the probability of a patient having a particular disease based on symptoms (evidence) and test results, starting from a baseline understanding of how common the disease is (prior).
  • Spam Filters: Email services use Bayesian filtering to determine if an email is spam. They learn from words and features commonly found in spam (evidence) to update the probability that a new email is spam.
  • Search Engines: Search algorithms use Bayesian methods to infer the relevance of web pages to your search query, updating rankings based on clicks and other user interactions (evidence).
  • Weather Forecasting: Meteorologists update the probability of rain based on current atmospheric conditions (evidence) and historical weather patterns (priors).
  • Legal Reasoning (informally): Jurors and investigators constantly update their beliefs about guilt or innocence as new pieces of evidence are presented during a trial. This tool aims to make that process more explicit.
  • Everyday Learning: When you try a new restaurant based on a friend's recommendation (evidence) and your prior experiences with their taste (prior), you're using a form of Bayesian updating.

This analyzer applies this powerful theorem to the evidence presented in the Karen Read trial, allowing you to see how individual pieces of information might systematically shift an initial belief.